Page 37 - Contributed Paper Session (CPS) - Volume 6
P. 37

CPS1483 Ken Karipidis et. al
            used, the RF energy absorption is at least several times higher in the temporal
            lobe than in the frontal lobe.(13) In our data there was a large number of
            gliomas with unspecified or overlapping location. Reclassification of these did
            reduce the trends for the temporal lobe during the periods before substantial
            mobile phone use, and for the frontal lobe during all the periods.
               In our study we also compared the observed incidence with a modelled
            predicted incidence assuming a causal association between mobile phone use
            and  glioma  as  reported  in  the  Interphone  and  Hardell  studies.  The  results
            suggest that, if the effects of mobile phones on glioma risk are real, then the
            incidence rates would be far higher than those observed. The present study
            has some limitations. The accuracy of the Australian cancer registration system
            in the early periods when it began in the 80s is unknown for all the states and
            territories.
               We  estimated  mobile  phone  use  using  information  on  mobile  phone
            accounts, and this may not be a true indicator of actual use as some people
            may have multiple accounts and others may use a phone without having an
            account.  We  mitigated  this  by  also  using  data  from  a  consumer  survey
            conducted by the national telecommunications regulator on the proportion of
            the population using mobile phones. Information from the survey was only
            available from the years 2009 to 2013 and this was applied to data on the
            annual number of mobile phone accounts from 1987. However, mobile phone
            use patterns have likely changed from 1987 to 2009. Further, the exposure
            metric is unclear when investigating whether mobile phone use is implicated
            in brain cancer risk. Prevalence of phone use is a de facto measure for the
            amount of RF energy a person is receiving when using a mobile phone, and
            changes  in  technology  and  patterns  of  individual  use  were  not  taken  into
            account in this investigation.
               We estimated the prevalence of mobile phone use equally across the 20-
            59 age range and both males and females. The use of subscription data in
            early  years  is  likely  to  underestimate  prevalence  of  use  in  males  and
            overestimate it in females given that users in early years were middle-aged
            working  men  on  company  mobile  phone  subscriptions.(14)  In  later  years
            mobile phone use became equal between the two genders.(15)
               For information on the proportion of regular and heavy mobile phone users
            we  used  data  from  the  Interphone  study,  which  also  included  data  from
            Australia. Mobile phone use in the Interphone study was self-reported, relying
            on  participants’  recall  of  past  phone  use.(2)  Sensitivity  analyses  on  the
            Interphone methodology reported that for short term recall (up to a year)
            there was underestimation of phone use by regular users and overestimation
            by  heavy  users.(16)  For  longer  recall  (3  to  5  years)  there  was  an
            underestimation of number of calls and an overestimation on the duration of



                                                                26 | I S I   W S C   2 0 1 9
   32   33   34   35   36   37   38   39   40   41   42